ISSN: 2641-2950
Journal of Neurology, Neurological Science and Disorders
Review Article       Open Access      Peer-Reviewed

The possible clinical correlation between preterm neurodevelopment and alterations in gut eubiosis

Giulio Perrotta*

Institute for the Study of Psychotherapies (I.S.P.), Via San Martino Della Battaglia N, 31, 00185, Rome, Italy
*Corresponding author: Giulio Perrotta, Institute for the Study of Psychotherapies (I.S.P.), Via San Martino Della Battaglia N, 31, 00185, Rome, Italy, Tel: +393492108872; E-mail: info@giulioperrotta.com
Received: 01 March, 2023 | Accepted: 26 April, 2023 | Published: 27 April, 2023
Keywords: Premature infants; Gut microbiota; Neurodevelopment; Psychologic development; Follow-up

Cite this as

Perrotta G (2023) The possible clinical correlation between preterm neurodevelopment and alterations in gut eubiosis. J Neurol Neurol Sci Disord 9(1): 014-021. DOI: 10.17352/jnnsd.000052

Copyright

© 2023 Perrotta G. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.

In the literature, the topic of neurocognitive development of preterm infants is of current interest. For more than a century, efforts have been made to study and demonstrate what factors may facilitate or interfere with normal neurodevelopment in preterm infants. Recently, attention seems to have focused on the role of gut microbiota. Several studies have shown that the cognitive performance of infants born preterm is lower in general and specific skills; behavior is also reduced in the first year of life; and, by school age, the cognitive development of infants born preterm is lower than that of full-term infants (these deficits are evident in the areas of learning, reading, writing, and mathematical skills, fine motor skills, communication, memory, and attention); however, there is currently no scientific evidence to confirm the existence of premature alterations in the gut microbiota concerning these morbid conditions, although there is evidence for the existence of the gut-brain axis and adverse outcomes on neurobiological function following physical afflictions, such as sepsis, necrotizing enterocolitis and other diseases. Supplementation of prebiotics and probiotics does not appear to correlate with improvement or worsening of future neurobiological and cognitive status, except in the ability to positively influence pathological conditions that indirectly may interfere with healthy neurodevelopmental outcomes of the premature infant.

Background

Introduction

In the scientific literature, the topic of neurocognitive and psychological development of preterm infants is of current interest. For more than a century, efforts have been made to study and demonstrate what factors are capable of facilitating or interfering with normal infant development, to prevent or correct any alterations or projectively predict developmental progress, identifying from time to time the possible factors impacting neurodevelopment, whether pathophysiological or pharmacological (during the gestation and developmental phase of the first three years of birth [1].

Generally, pathological neuroevolutionary findings are a consequence of cognitive (and psychiatric), motor, sensory (hearing, visual, tactile, olfactory, gustatory), neurological (epilepsy, hypoxic and/or ischemic consequences, oncological expansive processes and structural malformations), and genetic disorder [2,4]; such hypotheses may also be triggered or aggravated by stressogenic and algic factors, as well as nutritional and pharmacological [5,8].

Recently, several studies have supported the hypothesis of the correlation between gut dysbiosis and neurodevelopmental impairment in preterm infants [9]: “In preterm infants, predisposes him to various major morbidities including neonatal necrotizing enterocolitis and sepsis in the Neonatal Intensive Care Unit (NICU) and adverse neurological outcomes later in life. There are parallel early developmental windows for the gut microbiota and nervous system during prenatal to postnatal life. Therefore, preterm infants represent a unique population in which optimization of early colonization and microbiota development can influence brain development and improve neurological outcomes” [10]. However, beyond the suggestive hypothesis, there is no current scientific evidence confirming premature alterations of the gut microbiota with such morbid conditions, although there is evidence for the existence of the gut-brain axis and negative outcomes on neurobiological function as a result of physical afflictions, such as sepsis, necrotizing enterocolitis and other diseases. Supplementation of prebiotics and probiotics does not appear to correlate with improvement or worsening of future neurobiological and cognitive status, except to affect morbid conditions that indirectly may influence healthy neurodevelopmental outcomes of the premature infant [10].

Clinical examination and follow-up

During periods of hospitalization, caregiver intake during the developmental course, and follow-up appointments, identifying all factors impacting normal neurocognitive and psychological development of preterm infants in advance becomes critical if an extremely complicated clinical task, whether the caregiver’s intervention is clinician-assisted or to meet a scientific research need [11].

The neurocognitive examination is a fundamental part of the quality of care in Follow-up programs. It aims to identify and define major abnormalities early, enabling the implementation of the necessary early interventions, guiding the practitioner in communicating the diagnosis, directing support to parents and planning referral to territorial services for children with risk of disabilities or developmental disorders, sharing with the primary care pediatrician the problems encountered and possible solutions. A further objective, starting at 12 months of age, is to identify minor outcomes and further investigate the motor, neuropsychological, and behavioral development of preterm infants. Finally, for children who have developed cerebral palsy or major disabilities, the neuro-rehabilitation team has the task of verifying therapeutic interventions, providing guidance to parents regarding the prevention of musculoskeletal disorders, and sharing guidance on aids, autonomy, and prostheses with territorial services. The prognosis and evolution of clinical signs, but also the child’s and parents’ resilience pathways are not always easy to define, the lower the gestational ages [12].

Aims

The objective of the present work is to evaluate the current clinical evidence (e.g., clinical signs, anthropometrics, and instrumental findings) on the possible correlation between microbial alterations in the gut microbiota and neurodevelopment in the preterm infant, that is, the unborn who have a gestational age of fewer than 36 weeks or a weight of fewer than 1,500 grams.

Materials and methods

We searched Pubmed through December 31, 2022, clinical trials and randomized controlled trials using the keywords “gut microbiota”, “preterm infants”, “prebiotic” and “probiotic”, in combination, selecting 18 useful results. Simple reviews, opinion contributions, or publications in popular volumes were excluded because they were not relevant or redundant for this paper, as they did not present results or statistical samples but only protocol and research proposals, did not specifically address the relationship between the gut microbiota and preterm infants, the data were contradictory, unreliable, or otherwise, the research design had functional shortcomings, or the study sample was not directly preterm infants. Some reviews have been taken into account to justify the concluding assertions, consistent with the outcomes of studies on this research area. The search was not limited to English-language articles. No limit was placed on the year of publication.

Results

To evaluate indicators of quality of care in follow-up, a panel of experts from the same networks defined the areas and timing of evaluations [13].

The literature distinguishes between “major disabilities” and “minor dysfunctions”: in the first case fall the hypotheses of moderate or severe cerebral palsy with GMFCS (Gross motor function classification system) ≥ 2, cognitive scores at Bayley III < 70 and GMFCS ≥2, vision with deficits < 1/10 bilateral, permanent hearing impairment that does not allow the child to understand the directions of verbal messages and communicate despite prosthesis or cochlear implant; the second case includes hypotheses of disorders of the motor and postural sphere (clumsiness, clumsiness, coordination problems, organizational problems of movement and gesture, but also postural and morphological abnormalities at the level of the chest, spine, lower limbs and foot, plagiocephaly), difficulties in learning, visual-spatial and perceptual organization, behavioral disorders (adjustment disorders and disorders of the tonic-emotional sphere, hyperactivity or inhibition), and pathologies of adaptive functions (disorders of sleep-wake rhythm, feeding and sphincter control [14-16].

The studies conducted by Prechtl in the last thirty years have introduced into the clinical field the observation and qualitative evaluation of the spontaneous motility of the newborn (General Movements, GM), using ultrasound and ultrasound techniques (jerks, GM, isolated movements of the limbs, contractions, stretches, breathing movements, hiccups, yawning, turning and bending of the head, sucking and swallowing movements) (Table 1) [17].

Subsequent research has shown that GMs are an excellent indicator of early brain dysfunction, as they involve the whole body in a variable sequence of movements of the trunk, neck, legs, and arms and GMs in the preterm are similar to those of the fetus, as they are large, often fast and frequently accompanied by pelvic lift. The evaluation of GMs is based on the collection of video recordings and the subsequent analysis of the movement by properly trained personnel [18-21].

In recent decades, there has been an increasing need then to assess the communicative and language mental development of preterm birth within the first year of life to identify children with developmental problems/difficulties as early as possible and plan individualized habilitative and educational interventions. Many studies are showing a high incidence of developmental atypia in preterm low birth weight (VLBW) infants [22].

In the early years of a child’s life, motor, cognitive, communicative, and perceptual skills, and competencies are interdependent; we speak of “sensory and motor competencies” to emphasize the close overlap and interdependence of the motor, cognitive, perceptual, linguistic and communicative domains. The most widely used scales for assessing mental development in early childhood are the Bayley scales (with cognitive, motor, language, social-emotional, and adaptive behavioral subscales) and Griffiths (with locomotor, social, hearing-language, hand-eye coordination and performance subscales). Developmental scales are a standardized method of observing and assessing the developmental lines of a child’s behavior. The purpose of the scales is to assess the child’s current level of development and how far it deviates from the general population, intending to develop early habilitative programs. They do not measure an individual’s intelligence but assess the achievement of key developmental milestones (Figure 1) [23].

Cognitive assessment in early childhood is not aimed at determining an IQ. The tools intended for this age group are defined as “development tests” because they primarily evaluate the achievement of the fundamental stages of the growth path. Cognitive functions from 0 to 2 years being sensorimotor, are not separable or well differentiable. The Bayley-III provides a snapshot of the child’s development at a given moment and allows for the assessment of individual functional domains with flexible but highly standardized procedures. It consists of 5 scales, 3 direct administration scales and 2 scales aimed at parents. It is also accompanied by a report for the caregiver. The Cognitive scale consists of items written in such a way as to minimize the influence of children’s receptive language and motor skills in performing the task. The following are evaluated: sensorimotor development, exploration, and manipulation, memory, formation of concepts, the relationship between objects, and other aspects relating to the cognitive process. The motor scale assesses the child’s motor skills through two subscales: gross motor skills and fine motor skills. Fine-motor items assess praxis, motor-perceptual integration, planning, and motor speed. Gross motor items primarily measure limb and chest movement, assessing static positioning and dynamic movement, including locomotion and coordination, balance, and motor planning. The Language scale consists of two subscales: Receptive Communication and Expressive Communication (CE). Receptive communication items assess preverbal behavior; vocabulary development; the development of vocabulary related to the morphological aspect (e.g., pronouns and prepositions); understanding morphological indicators (e.g., plural “i” or “e”), tense indicators (e.g., “while” and gerund) and possessives; referential communication and verbal comprehension. Expressive Communication items assess preverbal communication (e.g., basic phonetics, gestural communication, references to conjunctions, respect for conversational turns); vocabulary development (e.g., naming objects, images and attributes, such as colors and sizes); morphosyntactic development (e.g., the use of expressions with two terms, plurals, and verb tenses). The Socioemotional scale evaluates the management of emotions, communication needs and the ability to relate to others. The items consider the domain of emotional-functional ability; the need for communication; commitment in relationships with others; the use of emotions for interactive purposes; use of emotional cues or gestures to solve problems. Finally, the Adaptive Behavior scale measures what the individual habitually does and what she may be able to do in some areas: Communication: speech, language, listening, and non-verbal communication skills; Preschool skills: letter recognition, counting, and drawing simple shapes; Self-control: following simple directions and orders and making choices; Game: play and follow rules; Social: getting along with other people, using good manners, helping others, and recognizing emotions; Community: interest in activities outside the home and recognition of different situations; Living at home: helping adults with household chores and taking care of their belongings; Health and safety: self-care and avoidance of physical hazards; Autonomy: eating, going to the bathroom and washing; Motor: movement and manipulation of objects. Scores for all skill areas combine to form a series of composites, including the Overall Adaptive Composite, which is an overall measure of a child’s adaptive development. The Griffiths III, on the other hand, develop along 5 scales, one less than the previous version with two main innovations: the Performance (E) and Ragionamento Pratico (F) scales have merged into a single Scale A (Fundamentals of learning) and Scale D (Personal-social-emotional) aspires to a much more sophisticated assessment which is not satisfied with assessing only adaptive behavior but aims at assessing the ability to read emotional expressions, empathy, theory of mind, self-awareness, self-judgment and morality. The age range taken into consideration (0 - 6 years) is narrower than the previous tool which was aimed at two age groups (0 - 2 and 2 - 8), considering the identification of possible disorders or developmental delays superfluous between the ages of 6 and 8.

The assessment of Developmental Quotient (QS) obtained from developmental scales differs from IQ and has a predictive purpose. Serious measurements of a child’s development over time can be made in the first 2 years of life. It is necessary to keep in mind that it is possible to detect variations in results between assessments that may be a sign of pathology or may be the result of measurement artifacts and/or physiological changes in the child’s performance. For prognostic purposes, it is useful to remember that physiological variations are also observable in children who are developing a disability, particularly mild-to-moderate disabilities. Some authors identify 2 years of corrected age as a key stage at which to conduct an assessment of cognitive development aimed at early identification of possible disabilities. At 2 years of corrected age, a broad assessment of the developmental domains: motor, cognitive (verbal and nonverbal), communicative, and relational can be conducted. The assessment of mental development in the preterm infant should be supplemented with detailed neuropsychological assessment, observation of regulatory skills, and attachment bonding [24-28].

In the literature, clinical studies focus on analyzing the correlations between defective neurodevelopment of preterm infants and serious events such as brain hemorrhage, sepsis and major respiratory, cardiac, and intestinal disorders (capable of interfering with normal brain function) and recently even the correlation with placental defects and neurological damage emerges [29] but the monitoring of risk factors decreases the negative impact on the patient’s health and neurodevelopment by almost 90% [30] including psychological profiles that in school age and adolescence detect the presence of anxious, depressive and hyperarousal behaviors [31-34].

All of these hypotheses have the potential to produce permanent impairment of normal neurocognitive and psychological development, which usually impacts severe forms of neurodevelopmental deficits in one out of three cases [35]; in particular, among children born extremely preterm, cerebral palsies and cranial abnormalities found on ultrasonography (and in those indicative of white matter damage) are predictive of neurodevelopmental impairment at age 10 years [36], but correlations have also been found between neurodevelopmental disorders and high hematocrit values, prolonged over time [37], without, however, finding an exact clinical correlation between this laboratory finding and the neurodevelopmental consequences.

On the subject of supplementation, on the other hand, there emerges a need for prior correction of disease profiles related to ai values of plasma iron [38,39] and folate deficiency [40], as well as an increased preference for fortified milk to improve the kinaesthetic performance of preterm infants [41], as well as the addition of vitamin A and enteral insulin to breast milk, under certain clinical hypotheses, positively correlated [42].

“Human milk has unique properties in promoting gastrointestinal maturation and immunological benefits, it is prudent to implement strategies to fortify it appropriately to achieve its benefits, including reduced rates of necrotizing enterocolitis, fewer episodes of sepsis and urinary tract infections, and improved visual and neurocognitive development” [43].

Discussion

However, such clinical evidence, relating precisely to the health status of preterm infants, is not directly reflected in any published studies on the correlation between unhealthy neurodevelopment and alterations in gut eubiosis; instead, there is evidence on the indirect effects of diseases caused or aggravated by gut dysbiosis, such as necrotizing enterocolitis and sepsis, as there is evidence of the brain-gut axis and the importance of the microbial role in our body [44].

The use of probiotics and prebiotics to regulate intestinal dysbiotic processes, in preterm infants, has not been significantly shown to play an important role in his neurodevelopment, apart from the positive effects related to the stabilization of eubiosis [45].

Correcting intestinal dysbiotic forms, especially in preterm infants, could promote proper microbial eubiosis of the whole organism, but such clinical operation does not seem to be correlated with direct damage to the normal neurodevelopment of the preterm infant, in the current state of medical knowledge, although there is evidence that alterations in the gut-brain axis can promote or aggravate many morbid conditions in adults [9]; however: “The impact of nutrition on brain development in preterm infants has been increasingly appreciated. Early postnatal growth and nutrient intake have been demonstrated to influence brain growth and maturation with subsequent effects on neurodevelopment that persist into childhood and adolescence. Nutrition could also potentially protect against injury. Inflammation and perinatal infection play a crucial role in the pathogenesis of white matter injury, the most common pattern of brain injury in preterm infants. Therefore, nutritional components with immunomodulatory and/or anti-inflammatory effects may serve as neuroprotective agents. Moreover, growing evidence supports the existence of a microbiome-gut-brain axis. The microbiome is thought to interact with the brain through immunological, endocrine, and neural pathways. Consequently, nutritional components that may influence gut microbiota may also exert beneficial effects on the developing brain. Based on these properties, probiotics, prebiotic oligosaccharides and certain amino acids are potential candidates for neuroprotection. In addition, the amino acid glutamine has been associated with a decrease in infectious morbidity in preterm infants. In conclusion, early postnatal nutrition is of major importance for brain growth and maturation. Additionally, certain nutritional components might play a neuroprotective role against white matter injury, through modulation of inflammation and infection, and may influence the microbiome-gut-brain axis” [46].

Finally, recent pharmacological findings rule out the correlation between neurodevelopmental deficits and pregnancy exposure to corticosteroids [47] and morphine (despite less recent contrary studies that also speak of a negative impact on growth and linear brain measures) [48-51], but confirm that combination therapy of inhaled nitric oxide and vitamin A in severely premature infants reduces the incidence of bronchopulmonary dysplasia and life-threatening complications, actually improving neurocognitive outcomes in the first year of life [52].

Understandably, the lower gestational age and presence of independent factors besides prematurity may lead to intellectual & developmental disabilities (you mentioned repeatedly). Worth noting is the perinatal and neonatal factors associated with improved outcomes. The long surveillance involves significant long-term medical challenges with major concerns of high-priced health care costs and the negative impact on the unprepared parents requiring repeated counseling on the complexity of the information. Several studies have shown that the cognitive performance of infants born preterm is lower in general and specific skills, such as visuoconstructive and visuoperceptual skills, receptive and expressive language, visuomotor integration, working memory and attention, gross and fine motor skills and social-emotional skills; exploratory and reaching behavior is also reduced in the first year of life. In fact, at school age, the cognitive development of children born preterm is lower than that of children born full term, except for special morbid conditions occurring later in life. These deficits are evident in the areas of learning, reading, writing and mathematics, fine motor skills, communication, memory and attention. There is also an increased presence of psychological problems such as anxiety, somatization, hyperactivity, oppositional defiant disorder, depression and socialization difficulties. Finally, in adolescence, data show that preterm children have an increased risk of hospitalization and general cognitive deficits, in verbal and visuomotor skills, executive functions such as inhibition and cognitive flexibility. Indeed, there is an increased risk of developing psychiatric disorders, particularly depression and anxiety disorders.

Limitations

No specific limitations were found, except for a few studies where an insufficient population sample size was reported. In addition, studies involving the intake of prebiotics and probiotics do not take into account their combined interaction, with and without drug therapy, and the nutritional and dietary profiles of patients, which therefore may override or diminish the efficacy of administration. Finally, the studies do not include an exact distinction between the pharmaceuticals used and the appropriate dosage, as well as the timing and duration of administration. These elements, insufficiently analyzed, may invalidate all or part of the outcome of the studies.

Conclusion

There is no conclusive clinical evidence that there is a direct correlation between gut dysbiosis and unhealthy neurodevelopment or that the use of probiotics and prebiotics enhances, reduces, or nurtures healthy neurocognitive development of preterm infants, although there is evidence of a correlation between various biological dysfunctions and altered gut eubiosis, especially in preterm infants, who from birth have a pattern of structural and functional alteration of Bifidi and other types of bacteria. In the future, it is suggested that more attention be paid to this study profile, and during clinical follow-ups consider the data to assess possible correlations between the maintenance of gut dysbiosis and adverse neurodevelopmental consequences in preterm infants, in order also to facilitate early intervention to support various cognitive, motor and language skills and any developmental delays that might create limitations in the performance of normal daily activities.

  1. Marret S, Marchand-Martin L, Picaud JC, Hascoët JM, Arnaud C, Rozé JC, Truffert P, Larroque B, Kaminski M, Ancel PY; EPIPAGE Study Group. Brain injury in very preterm children and neurosensory and cognitive disabilities during childhood: the EPIPAGE cohort study. PLoS One. 2013 May 2;8(5):e62683. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0062683. PMID: 23658763; PMCID: PMC3642195.
  2. Moore T, Hennessy EM, Myles J, Johnson SJ, Draper ES, Costeloe KL, Marlow N. Neurological and developmental outcome in extremely preterm children born in England in 1995 and 2006: the EPICure studies. BMJ. 2012 Dec 4;345:e7961. doi: 10.1136/bmj.e7961. PMID: 23212880; PMCID: PMC3514471.
  3. Delobel-Ayoub M, Arnaud C, White-Koning M, Casper C, Pierrat V, Garel M, Burguet A, Roze JC, Matis J, Picaud JC, Kaminski M, Larroque B; EPIPAGE Study Group. Behavioral problems and cognitive performance at 5 years of age after very preterm birth: the EPIPAGE Study. Pediatrics. 2009 Jun;123(6):1485-92. doi: 10.1542/peds.2008-1216. PMID: 19482758.
  4. Pierrat V, Marchand-Martin L, Guemas I, Matis J, Burguet A, Picaud JC, Fresson J, Alberge C, Marret S, Roze JC, Kaminski M, Larroque B, Ancel PY; Epipage Study Group. Height at 2 and 5 years of age in children born very preterm: the EPIPAGE study. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed. 2011 Sep;96(5):F348-54. doi: 10.1136/adc.2010.185470. Epub 2011 Jan 17. PMID: 21242241.
  5. Wilson-Costello D, Friedman H, Minich N, Siner B, Taylor G, Schluchter M, Hack M. Improved neurodevelopmental outcomes for extremely low birth weight infants in 2000-2002. Pediatrics. 2007 Jan;119(1):37-45. doi: 10.1542/peds.2006-1416. PMID: 17200269.
  6. Lapillonne A, O'Connor DL, Wang D, Rigo J. Nutritional recommendations for the late-preterm infant and the preterm infant after hospital discharge. J Pediatr. 2013 Mar;162(3 Suppl):S90-100. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2012.11.058. PMID: 23445854.
  7. Zachariassen G, Faerk J, Grytter C, Esberg BH, Hjelmborg J, Mortensen S, Thybo Christesen H, Halken S. Nutrient enrichment of mother's milk and growth of very preterm infants after hospital discharge. Pediatrics. 2011 Apr;127(4):e995-e1003. doi: 10.1542/peds.2010-0723. Epub 2011 Mar 14. PMID: 21402642.
  8. Young L, Embleton ND, McCormick FM, McGuire W. Multinutrient fortification of human breast milk for preterm infants following hospital discharge. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2013 Feb 28;2013(2):CD004866. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD004866.pub4. PMID: 23450556; PMCID: PMC8855689.
  9. Perrotta G. The intestinal microbiota: towards a multifactorial integrative model. Eubiosis and dysbiosis in morbid physical and psychological conditions. Arch Clin Gastroenterol 2021; 7(2): 024-035. DOI: 10.17352/2455-2283.000094.
  10. Lu J, Claud EC. Connection between gut microbiome and brain development in preterm infants. Dev Psychobiol. 2019 Jul;61(5):739-751. doi: 10.1002/dev.21806. Epub 2018 Nov 20. PMID: 30460694; PMCID: PMC6728148.
  11. de Kieviet JF, Piek JP, Aarnoudse-Moens CS, Oosterlaan J. Motor development in very preterm and very low-birth-weight children from birth to adolescence: a meta-analysis. JAMA. 2009 Nov 25;302(20):2235-42. doi: 10.1001/jama.2009.1708. PMID: 19934425.
  12. Wright LL. What will it take to improve very low birth weight follow-up care? Pediatrics. 2006 Jun;117(6):2277-8. doi: 10.1542/peds.2005-2886. PMID: 16740874.
  13. Wang CJ, McGlynn EA, Brook RH, Leonard CH, Piecuch RE, Hsueh SI, Schuster MA. Quality-of-care indicators for the neurodevelopmental follow-up of very low birth weight children: results of an expert panel process. Pediatrics. 2006 Jun;117(6):2080-92. doi: 10.1542/peds.2005-1904. PMID: 16740851.
  14. Ment LR, Vohr B, Allan W, Katz KH, Schneider KC, Westerveld M, Duncan CC, Makuch RW. Change in cognitive function over time in very low-birth-weight infants. JAMA. 2003 Feb 12;289(6):705-11. doi: 10.1001/jama.289.6.705. PMID: 12585948.
  15. Cooke RW, Foulder-Hughes L. Growth impairment in the very preterm and cognitive and motor performance at 7 years. Arch Dis Child. 2003 Jun;88(6):482-7. doi: 10.1136/adc.88.6.482. PMID: 12765911; PMCID: PMC1763118.
  16. Msall ME. Neurodevelopmental surveillance in the first 2 years after extremely preterm birth: evidence, challenges, and guidelines. Early Hum Dev. 2006 Mar;82(3):157-66. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2005.12.016. Epub 2006 Mar 10. PMID: 16530359.
  17. Prechtl HF. Qualitative changes of spontaneous movements in fetus and preterm infant are a marker of neurological dysfunction. Early Hum Dev. 1990 Sep;23(3):151-8. doi: 10.1016/0378-3782(90)90011-7. PMID: 2253578.
  18. Ferrari F, Cioni G, Prechtl HF. Qualitative changes of general movements in preterm infants with brain lesions. Early Hum Dev. 1990 Sep;23(3):193-231. doi: 10.1016/0378-3782(90)90013-9. PMID: 2253580.
  19. Cioni G, Ferrari F, Einspieler C, Paolicelli PB, Barbani MT, Prechtl HF. Comparison between observation of spontaneous movements and neurologic examination in preterm infants. J Pediatr. 1997 May;130(5):704-11. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3476(97)80010-8. PMID: 9152277.
  20. Prechtl HF, Einspieler C, Cioni G, Bos AF, Ferrari F, Sontheimer D. An early marker for neurological deficits after perinatal brain lesions. Lancet. 1997 May 10;349(9062):1361-3. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(96)10182-3. PMID: 9149699.
  21. Einspieler C, Prechtl HF, Ferrari F, Cioni G, Bos AF. The qualitative assessment of general movements in preterm, term and young infants--review of the methodology. Early Hum Dev. 1997 Nov 24;50(1):47-60. doi: 10.1016/s0378-3782(97)00092-3. PMID: 9467693.
  22. Aarnoudse-Moens CS, Weisglas-Kuperus N, van Goudoever JB, Oosterlaan J. Meta-analysis of neurobehavioral outcomes in very preterm and/or very low birth weight children. Pediatrics. 2009 Aug;124(2):717-28. doi: 10.1542/peds.2008-2816. Epub 2009 Jul 27. PMID: 19651588.
  23. Pugliese M, Rossi C, Guidotti I, Gallo C, Della Casa E, Bertoncelli N, Coccolini E, Ferrari F. Preterm birth and developmental problems in infancy and preschool age Part II: cognitive, neuropsychological and behavioural outcomes. J Matern Fetal Neonatal Med. 2013 Nov;26(16):1653-7. doi: 10.3109/14767058.2013.794205. Epub 2013 May 9. PMID: 23570550.
  24. Baron IS, Leonberger KA. Assessment of intelligence in the preschool period. Neuropsychol Rev. 2012 Dec;22(4):334-44. doi: 10.1007/s11065-012-9215-0. Epub 2012 Oct 11. PMID: 23054100.
  25. Arpi E, Ferrari F. Preterm birth and behaviour problems in infants and preschool-age children: a review of the recent literature. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2013 Sep;55(9):788-96. doi: 10.1111/dmcn.12142. Epub 2013 Mar 21. PMID: 23521214.
  26. Ferrari F, Gallo C, Pugliese M, Guidotti I, Gavioli S, Coccolini E, Zagni P, Della Casa E, Rossi C, Lugli L, Todeschini A, Ori L, Bertoncelli N. Preterm birth and developmental problems in the preschool age. Part I: minor motor problems. J Matern Fetal Neonatal Med. 2012 Nov;25(11):2154-9. doi: 10.3109/14767058.2012.696164. Epub 2012 Jun 19. PMID: 22630565.
  27. Perrotta G. Neonatal and infantile abuse in a family setting. Open J Pediatr Child Health 2020; 5(1): 034-042. DOI: 10.17352/ojpch.000028.
  28. Perrotta G, Fabiano G. Behavioural disorders in children and adolescents: Definition, clinical contexts, neurobiological profiles, and clinical treatments. Open J Pediatr Child Health 2021; 6(1): 005-015. DOI: 10.17352/ ojpch.000030.
  29. Santos HP Jr, Bhattacharya A, Joseph RM, Smeester L, Kuban KCK, Marsit CJ, O'Shea TM, Fry RC. Evidence for the placenta-brain axis: multi-omic kernel aggregation predicts intellectual and social impairment in children born extremely preterm. Mol Autism. 2020 Dec 11;11(1):97. doi: 10.1186/s13229-020-00402-w. PMID: 33308293; PMCID: PMC7730750.
  30. Schmid MB, Reister F, Mayer B, Hopfner RJ, Fuchs H, Hummler HD. Prospective risk factor monitoring reduces intracranial hemorrhage rates in preterm infants. Dtsch Arztebl Int. 2013 Jul;110(29-30):489-96. doi: 10.3238/arztebl.2013.0489. Epub 2013 Jul 22. PMID: 24000297; PMCID: PMC3752580.
  31. Frazier JA, Cochran D, Kim S, Jalnapurkar I, Joseph RM, Hooper SR, Santos HP Jr, Ru H, Venuti L, Singh R, Washburn LK, Gogcu S, Msall ME, Kuban KCK, Rollins JV, Hanson SG, Jara H, Pastyrnak SL, Roell KR, Fry RC, O'Shea TM; ELGAN Study Investigators. Psychiatric Outcomes, Functioning, and Participation in Extremely Low Gestational Age Newborns at Age 15 Years. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2022 Jul;61(7):892-904.e2. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2021.12.008. Epub 2021 Dec 29. PMID: 34973366; PMCID: PMC9240104.
  32. Frazier JA, Cochran D, Kim S, Jalnapurkar I, Joseph RM, Hooper SR, Santos HP Jr, Ru H, Venuti L, Singh R, Washburn LK, Gogcu S, Msall ME, Kuban KCK, Rollins JV, Hanson SG, Jara H, Pastyrnak SL, Roell KR, Fry RC, O'Shea TM; ELGAN Study Investigators. Psychiatric Outcomes, Functioning, and Participation in Extremely Low Gestational Age Newborns at Age 15 Years. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2022 Jul;61(7):892-904.e2. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2021.12.008. Epub 2021 Dec 29. PMID: 34973366; PMCID: PMC9240104.
  33. Perrotta G. Anxiety disorders: definitions, contexts, neural correlates, and strategic therapy. J Neurol Neurosci. 2019; 6(1):046.
  34. Ai Y, Zhao J, Shi J, Zhu TT. Antibiotic exposure and childhood attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2021 Nov;238(11):3055-3062. doi: 10.1007/s00213-021-05989-3. Epub 2021 Oct 23. PMID: 34687335.
  35. Taylor GL, Joseph RM, Kuban KCK, Douglass LM, Laux J, Andrews B, Fry RC, Price WA, O'Shea TM. Changes in Neurodevelopmental Outcomes From Age 2 to 10 Years for Children Born Extremely Preterm. Pediatrics. 2021 May;147(5):e2020001040. doi: 10.1542/peds.2020-001040. Epub 2021 Apr 6. PMID: 33824183; PMCID: PMC8086004.
  36. Campbell H, Check J, Kuban KCK, Leviton A, Joseph RM, Frazier JA, Douglass LM, Roell K, Allred EN, Fordham LA, Hooper SR, Jara H, Paneth N, Mokrova I, Ru H, Santos HP Jr, Fry RC, O'Shea TM. Neonatal Cranial Ultrasound Findings among Infants Born Extremely Preterm: Associations with Neurodevelopmental Outcomes at 10 Years of Age. J Pediatr. 2021 Oct;237:197-205.e4. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2021.05.059. Epub 2021 Jun 4. PMID: 34090894; PMCID: PMC8478718.
  37. McCoy TE, Conrad AL, Richman LC, Lindgren SD, Nopoulos PC, Bell EF. Neurocognitive profiles of preterm infants randomly assigned to lower or higher hematocrit thresholds for transfusion. Child Neuropsychol. 2011;17(4):347-67. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2010.544647. PMID: 21360360; PMCID: PMC3115491.`
  38. Steinmacher J, Pohlandt F, Bode H, Sander S, Kron M, Franz AR. Randomized trial of early versus late enteral iron supplementation in infants with a birth weight of less than 1301 grams: neurocognitive development at 5.3 years' corrected age. Pediatrics. 2007 Sep;120(3):538-46. doi: 10.1542/peds.2007-0495. PMID: 17766527.
  39. Peltoniemi OM, Anttila E, Kaukola T, Buonocore G, Hallman M. Randomized trial of early erythropoietin supplementation after preterm birth: Iron metabolism and outcome. Early Hum Dev. 2017 Jun;109:44-49. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2017.04.001. Epub 2017 Apr 20. PMID: 28433798.
  40. Li Z, Mei Z, Zhang L, Li H, Zhang Y, Li N, Ye R, Ren A, Liu JM, Serdula MK. Effects of Prenatal Micronutrient Supplementation on Spontaneous Preterm Birth: A Double-Blind Randomized Controlled Trial in China. Am J Epidemiol. 2017 Aug 1;186(3):318-325. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwx094. PMID: 28472219; PMCID: PMC9035906.
  41. Blakstad EW, Strømmen K, Moltu SJ, Wattam-Bell J, Nordheim T, Almaas AN, Grønn M, Rønnestad AE, Brække K, Iversen PO, von Hofsten C, Veierød MB, Westerberg AC, Drevon CA, Nakstad B. Improved Visual Perception in Very Low Birth Weight Infants on Enhanced Nutrient Supply. Neonatology. 2015;108(1):30-7. doi: 10.1159/000381660. Epub 2015 May 6. PMID: 25967892.
  42. Mank E, Naninck EFG, Limpens J, van Toledo L, van Goudoever JB, van den Akker CHP. Enteral Bioactive Factor Supplementation in Preterm Infants: A Systematic Review. Nutrients. 2020 Sep 24;12(10):2916. doi: 10.3390/nu12102916. PMID: 32987621; PMCID: PMC7598610.
  43. Bhatia J. Human milk and the premature infant. Ann Nutr Metab. 2013;62 Suppl 3:8-14. doi: 10.1159/000351537. Epub 2013 Aug 19. PMID: 23970211.
  44. Upadhyay RP, Taneja S, Chowdhury R, Strand TA, Bhandari N. Effect of prebiotic and probiotic supplementation on neurodevelopment in preterm very low birth weight infants: findings from a meta-analysis. Pediatr Res. 2020 Apr;87(5):811-822. doi: 10.1038/s41390-018-0211-9. Epub 2018 Oct 18. PMID: 30353041.
  45. Wu W, Zhao A, Liu B, Ye WH, Su HW, Li J, Zhang YM. Neurodevelopmental Outcomes and Gut Bifidobacteria in Term Infants Fed an Infant Formula Containing High sn-2 Palmitate: A Cluster Randomized Clinical Trial. Nutrients. 2021 Feb 22;13(2):693. doi: 10.3390/nu13020693. PMID: 33671493; PMCID: PMC7926808.
  46. Keunen K, van Elburg RM, van Bel F, Benders MJ. Impact of nutrition on brain development and its neuroprotective implications following preterm birth. Pediatr Res. 2015 Jan;77(1-2):148-55. doi: 10.1038/pr.2014.171. Epub 2014 Oct 14. PMID: 25314585; PMCID: PMC4291511.
  47. Wapner RJ, Sorokin Y, Mele L, Johnson F, Dudley DJ, Spong CY, Peaceman AM, Leveno KJ, Malone F, Caritis SN, Mercer B, Harper M, Rouse DJ, Thorp JM, Ramin S, Carpenter MW, Gabbe SG; National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Maternal-Fetal Medicine Units Network. Long-term outcomes after repeat doses of antenatal corticosteroids. N Engl J Med. 2007 Sep 20;357(12):1190-8. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa071453. PMID: 17881751.
  48. van den Bosch GE, White T, El Marroun H, Simons SH, van der Lugt A, van der Geest JN, Tibboel D, van Dijk M. Prematurity, Opioid Exposure and Neonatal Pain: Do They Affect the Developing Brain? Neonatology. 2015;108(1):8-15. doi: 10.1159/000376566. Epub 2015 Apr 11. PMID: 25871803.
  49. Whitfield MF, Grunau RE. Behavior, pain perception, and the extremely low-birth weight survivor. Clin Perinatol. 2000 Jun;27(2):363-79. doi: 10.1016/s0095-5108(05)70026-9. PMID: 10863655.
  50. Ferguson SA, Ward WL, Paule MG, Hall RW, Anand KJ. A pilot study of preemptive morphine analgesia in preterm neonates: effects on head circumference, social behavior, and response latencies in early childhood. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2012 Jan-Feb;34(1):47-55. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2011.10.008. Epub 2011 Nov 7. PMID: 22094261.
  51. Gadhia MM, Cutter GR, Abman SH, Kinsella JP. Effects of early inhaled nitric oxide therapy and vitamin A supplementation on the risk for bronchopulmonary dysplasia in premature newborns with respiratory failure. J Pediatr. 2014 Apr;164(4):744-8. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2013.11.040. Epub 2013 Dec 31. PMID: 24388327; PMCID: PMC3962699.
  52. Ramsay M, Fitzhardinge PM. A comparative study of two developmental scales: the Bayley and the Griffiths. Early Hum Dev. 1977 Oct;1(2):151-7. doi: 10.1016/0378-3782(77)90016-0. PMID: 617307.
 

Help ?